2020.11.01;天Nov1st(306):Memory failure predicted by attention lapsing and media multitasking | Nature
Published by DB,
Abstract
With the explosion of digital media and technologies, scholars, educators and the public have become increasingly vocal about the role that an ‘attention economy’ has in our lives1. The rise of the current digital culture coincides with longstanding scientific questions about why humans sometimes remember and sometimes forget, and why some individuals remember better than others2,3,4,5,6. Here we examine whether spontaneous attention lapses—in the moment7,8,9,10,11,12, across individuals13,14,15 and as a function of everyday media multitasking16,17,18,19—negatively correlate with remembering. Electroencephalography and pupillometry measures of attention20,21 were recorded as eighty young adults (mean age, 21.7 years) performed a goal-directed episodic encoding and retrieval task22. Trait-level sustained attention was further quantified using task-based23 and questionnaire measures24,25. Using trial-to-trial retrieval data, we show that tonic lapses in attention in the moment before remembering, assayed by posterior alpha power and pupil diameter, were correlated with reductions in neural signals of goal coding and memory, along with behavioural forgetting. Independent measures of trait-level attention lapsing mediated the relationship between neural assays of lapsing and memory performance, and between media multitasking and memory. Attention lapses partially account for why we remember or forget in the moment, and why some individuals remember better than others. Heavier media multitasking is associated with a propensity to have attention lapses and forget.
Access options
Subscribe to Journal
Get full journal access for 1 year
$199.00
only $3.90 per issue
All prices are NET prices.
VAT will be added later in the checkout.
Rent or Buy article
Get time limited or full article access on ReadCube.
from$8.99
All prices are NET prices.
Additional access options:
Data availability
Data that support the findings of this study are publicly available via the Open Science Framework47 with identifier zj7tb (https://osf.io/zj7tb). Data used in the preparation of this manuscript are also publicly available from the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Data Archive (NDA) (https://doi.org/10.15154/1519022)48. The source data underlying all figures are provided as a Source Data file. Source data are provided with this paper.
Code availability
Analytic code that support the findings of this study are publicly available via Open Science Framework47 with identifier zj7tb (https://osf.io/zj7tb).
References
Harris, T. Optimizing for Engagement: Understanding the Use of Persuasive Technology on Internet Platforms. Testimony on behalf of Center for Humane Technology. https://www.commerce.senate.gov/2019/6/optimizing-for-engagement-understanding-the-use-of-persuasive- technology-on-internet-platforms (US Senate Committee on Commerce, Science & Transportation; 25 June 2019).
Baddeley, A., Lewis, V., Eldridge, M. & Thomson, N. Attention and retrieval from long-term memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 113, 518–540 (1984).
Craik, F. I., Govoni, R., Naveh-Benjamin, M. & Anderson, N. D. The effects of divided attention on encoding and retrieval processes in human memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 125, 159–180 (1996).
Anderson, M. C. & Spellman, B. A. On the status of inhibitory mechanisms in cognition: memory retrieval as a model case. Psychol. Rev. 102, 68–100 (1995).
Evans, L. H. & Herron, J. E. Pre-retrieval event-related potentials predict source memory during task switching. Neuroimage 194, 174–181 (2019).
Curran, T. Effects of attention and confidence on the hypothesized ERP correlates of recollection and familiarity. Neuropsychologia 42, 1088–1106 (2004).
Klimesch, W. α-Band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 606–617 (2012).
Hanslmayr, S., Staudigl, T. & Fellner, M. C. Oscillatory power decreases and long-term memory: the information via desynchronization hypothesis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6, 74 (2012).
Jin, C. Y., Borst, J. P. & van Vugt, M. K. Predicting task-general mind-wandering with EEG. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 19, 1059–1073 (2019).
Unsworth, N. & Robison, M. K. The importance of arousal for variation in working memory capacity and attention control: a latent variable pupillometry study. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 43, 1962–1987 (2017).
Unsworth, N. & Robison, M. K. Pupillary correlates of lapses of sustained attention. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 16, 601–615 (2016).
Konishi, M., Brown, K., Battaglini, L. & Smallwood, J. When attention wanders: pupillometric signatures of fluctuations in external attention. Cognition 168, 16–26 (2017).
Fortenbaugh, F. C., DeGutis, J. & Esterman, M. Recent theoretical, neural, and clinical advances in sustained attention research. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 1396, 70–91 (2017).
Unsworth, N. Individual differences in long-term memory. Psychol. Bull. 145, 79–139 (2019).
Kahana, M. J., Aggarwal, E. V. & Phan, T. D. The variability puzzle in human memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 44, 1857–1863 (2018).
Uncapher, M. R. & Wagner, A. D. Minds and brains of media multitaskers: current findings and future directions. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 115, 9889–9896 (2018).
Ralph, B. C. W., Thomson, D. R., Seli, P., Carriere, J. S. & Smilek, D. Media multitasking and behavioral measures of sustained attention. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 77, 390–401 (2015).
Uncapher, M. R., K Thieu, M. & Wagner, A. D. Media multitasking and memory: differences in working memory and long-term memory. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 23, 483–490 (2016).
Baumgartner, S. E., van der Schuur, W. A., Lemmens, J. S. & te Poel, F. The relationship between media multitasking and attention problems in adolescents: results of two longitudinal studies. Hum. Commun. Res. 44, 3–30 (2018).
Macdonald, J. S. P., Mathan, S. & Yeung, N. Trial-by-trial variations in subjective attentional state are reflected in ongoing prestimulus EEG alpha oscillations. Front. Psychol. 2, 82 (2011).
Unsworth, N. & Robison, M. K. Tracking arousal state and mind wandering with pupillometry. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 18, 638–664 (2018).
Dobbins, I. G. & Wagner, A. D. Domain-general and domain-specific prefrontal mechanisms for recollecting events and detecting novelty. Cereb. Cortex 29, 150–166 (2005).
Esterman, M., Noonan, S. K., Rosenberg, M. & Degutis, J. In the zone or zoning out? Tracking behavioral and neural fluctuations during sustained attention. Cereb. Cortex 23, 2712–2723 (2013).
Ophir, E., Nass, C. & Wagner, A. D. Cognitive control in media multitaskers. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 106, 15583–15587 (2009).
Ralph, B. C. W., Thomson, D. R., Cheyne, J. A. & Smilek, D. Media multitasking and failures of attention in everyday life. Psychol. Res. 78, 661–669 (2014).
Herron, J. E. & Evans, L. H. Preparation breeds success: brain activity predicts remembering. Cortex 106, 1–11 (2018).
Forstmann, B. U., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Kaiser, J. & Bledowski, C. At your own peril: an ERP study of voluntary task set selection processes in the medial frontal cortex. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7, 286–296 (2007).
Rugg, M. D. & Curran, T. Event-related potentials and recognition memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 251–257 (2007).
Ra, C. K. et al. Association of digital media use with subsequent symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among adolescents. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 320, 255–263 (2018).
Hanslmayr, S. et al. The relationship between brain oscillations and BOLD signal during memory formation: a combined EEG-fMRI study. J. Neurosci. 31, 15674–15680 (2011).
Kessler, R. C. et al. The World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychol. Med. 35, 245–256 (2005).
Patton, J. H., Stanford, M. S. & Barratt, E. S. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J. Clin. Psychol. 51, 768–774 (1995).
Green, C. S. & Bavelier, D. Action-video-game experience alters the spatial resolution of vision. Psychol. Sci. 18, 88–94 (2007).
Carriere, J. S. A., Seli, P. & Smilek, D. Wandering in both mind and body: individual differences in mind wandering and inattention predict fidgeting. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 67, 19–31 (2013).
Carriere, J. S. A., Cheyne, J. A. & Smilek, D. Everyday attention lapses and memory failures: the affective consequences of mindlessness. Conscious. Cogn. 17, 835–847 (2008).
Brainard, D. H. The psychophysics toolbox. Spat. Vis. 10, 433–436 (1997).
Willenbockel, V. et al. Controlling low-level image properties: the SHINE toolbox. Behav. Res. Methods 42, 671–684 (2010).
Unsworth, N., Robison, M. K. & Miller, A. L. Pupillary correlates of fluctuations in sustained attention. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 30, 1241–1253 (2018).
Delorme, A. & Makeig, S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21 (2004).
Green, D. M. & Swets, J. A. Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics (Wiley, 1966).
Kleifges, K., Bigdely-Shamlo, N., Kerick, S. E. & Robbins, K. A. BLINKER: automated extraction of ocular indices from EEG enabling large-scale analysis. Front. Neurosci. 11, 12 (2017).
Goldinger, S. D. & Papesh, M. H. Pupil dilation reflects the creation and retrieval of memories. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 21, 90–95 (2012).
Otero, S. C., Weekes, B. S. & Hutton, S. B. Pupil size changes during recognition memory. Psychophysiology 48, 1346–1353 (2011).
Võ, M. L.-H. et al. The coupling of emotion and cognition in the eye: introducing the pupil old/new effect. Psychophysiology 45, 130–140 (2008).
Hong, L., Walz, J. M. & Sajda, P. Your eyes give you away: prestimulus changes in pupil diameter correlate with poststimulus task-related EEG dynamics. PLoS ONE 9, e91321 (2014).
Rosenberg, M., Noonan, S., DeGutis, J. & Esterman, M. Sustaining visual attention in the face of distraction: a novel gradual-onset continuous performance task. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 75, 426–439 (2013).
Madore, K. P. Memory failure predicted by attention lapsing and media multitasking (dataset and analytic code). https://osf.io/zj7tb (Open Science Framework, 2020).
Madore, K. P. Memory failure predicted by attention lapsing and media multitasking (dataset). https://doi.org/10.15154/1519022 (National Institute of Mental Health Data Archive, 2020).
Acknowledgements
We thank A. Gonzalez and J. Qi for assistance with various aspects of the study. This research was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (R56MH111672 to A.D.W.) and the National Institute on Aging (R01AG065255 to A.D.W.; F32AG059341 to K.P.M.). The content is solely the views of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.
Author information
Affiliations
Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
Kevin P. Madore, Anna M. Khazenzon, Jiefeng Jiang, Anthony M. Norcia & Anthony D. Wagner
Symbolic Systems Program, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
Cameron W. Backes
Department of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA
Melina R. Uncapher
Neuroscape, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA
Melina R. Uncapher
Wu Tsai Neurosciences Institute, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
Anthony M. Norcia & Anthony D. Wagner
Contributions
K.P.M., A.M.K., M.R.U., A.M.N. and A.D.W. developed the study concept and contributed to study design; K.P.M. and C.W.B. collected data under supervision of A.M.N. and A.D.W.; K.P.M., A.M.K., C.W.B. and J.J. analysed data under supervision of A.M.N. and A.D.W.; K.P.M. and A.D.W. wrote the manuscript, and all authors provided critical revisions.
Corresponding authors
Correspondence to Kevin P. Madore or Anthony D. Wagner.
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Peer review information Nature thanks Edward K. Vogel and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work.
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Extended data figures and tables
Extended Data Fig. 1 Experimental design.
a, Schematic of the goal-directed memory task with EEG and pupillometry measurements. b, Schematic of electrode clusters from which alpha or ERP signals were extracted for the respective analyses; electrode clusters are illustrated on a 128-channel net. Pupil diameter from the right eye (top right) was recorded concurrently using an eye-tracking system. L, left; R, right.
Extended Data Fig. 2 Pre-goal attention lapses relate to canonical neural signals of recollection- and familiarity-based memory as assayed by grand-average left-lateralized Parietal Old/New and FN400 ERP effects, respectively.
a, Evidence of a peak Parietal Old/New signal (indicated by the black arrow) in the 500–600-ms post-probe window as a function of memory outcome in conceptual and perceptual source-retrieval trials. b, Trial-level interaction between pre-goal attention lapses and the Parietal Old/New signal during remembered (source hit) and forgotten (miss) trials. c, Evidence of a peak FN400 signal (indicated by the black arrow) in the 400–500-ms post-probe window as a function of memory outcome in novelty-detection trials. d, Trial-level interaction between pre-goal attention lapses and FN400 signal on correctly endorsed new items (hits) compared with misses. For visualization, quintiles are shown for the relationship between pre-goal lapsing and ERP signal; statistics included an interaction term for retrieval goal state (for Parietal Old/New) and treated pre-goal lapsing and the ERP signals continuously in trial-level mixed models. y-axis units are z-scores. Data are mean ± s.e.m. Note that z-scoring within run and time-binning in 0.1-s (100-ms) intervals reduces smaller temporal effects that are sometimes exhibited in grand-average ERP plots (for visualization of grand-average ERP plots downsampled to 0.01-s intervals (10-ms), see Extended Data Fig. 4). CRold, correct rejection of old item; CRnew, correct rejection of new item; FAold, false alarm to old item; FAnew, false alarm to new item. n = 75 participants from a single independent experiment. Source data
Extended Data Fig. 3 Evidence of mean peak Parietal Old/New signal in the 500–600-ms post-probe window as a function of memory outcome in source retrieval trials.
The mean peak Parietal Old/New signal is indicated by the black arrow. a, b, Data are split by conceptual (a) and perceptual (b) source trials. CRold, correct rejection of old item; FAold, false alarm to old item. For conceptual cuing, hits and misses are for conceptually studied items, and correct rejections and false alarms are for perceptually studied items. For perceptual cuing, hits and misses are for perceptually studied items, and correct rejections and false alarms are for conceptually studied items. n = 75 participants from a single independent experiment. Source data
Extended Data Fig. 4 Grand-average left-lateralized ERPs revealing recollection-based Parietal Old/New and familiarity-based FN400 memory effects.
Data were down-sampled to 10-ms time-bin intervals. a, b, The same profile of findings is observed as with the 100-ms time-bins (see main text), such that evidence of a peak Parietal Old/New signal (indicated by the black arrow) is exhibited 500–600-ms post-probe onset as a function of memory outcome in conceptual and perceptual source-retrieval trials (a) and evidence of a peak FN400 signal (indicated by the black arrow) is exhibited 400–500-ms post-probe onset as a function of memory outcome in novelty-detection trials (b). y-axis units are within-run z-scores. n = 75 participants from a single independent experiment. Source data
Extended Data Fig. 5 Trait-level differences in sustained attention at encoding help to explain why individuals are more prone to remembering or forgetting.
a, b, Greater pre-goal attention lapsing at encoding is correlated with greater pre-goal attention lapsing at retrieval (a) and lower d′ on the memory task (b). For visualization, raw scores are plotted; statistics included z-scored assays with Pearson correlations. n = 75 participants for alpha retrieval data and n = 80 participants for all other data from a single independent experiment. These trait differences in attention at encoding do not fully explain the relationship between the trait differences in attention at retrieval and memory ability (Supplementary Information). Source data
Extended Data Fig. 6 Phasic pupil and memory effects.
Evidence of a phasic pupil old/new effect in novelty-detection trials 300–500 ms post-probe, particularly between correctly rejected old objects versus hits to new objects. The mean peak difference is at 400 ms post-probe (indicated by the black arrow). x-axis units are 100-ms time-bin intervals; y-axis units are within-run z-scores. n = 75 participants from a single independent experiment. Source data
Extended Data Fig. 7 Key results from extreme group analyses of multitasking, memory and sustained attention for light and heavy media multitaskers.
a–c, Heavy media multitaskers exhibited lower d′ on the memory tasks (a), more attention lapses on the gradCPT (b) and more evidence of attention lapsing (assayed by mean alpha power and pupil variability) on the memory task (c), relative to light media multitaskers. Data are mean ± s.e.m. from a single independent experiment. n = 18 light and n = 18 heavy media multitaskers for alpha data; n = 20 light and n = 20 heavy media multitaskers for all other data. d, Histogram of scores (n = 80) on the MMI, illustrated by the bottom 25% of scores (light media multitaskers), the middle 50% of scores (intermediate media multitaskers) and the top 25% of scores (heavy media multitaskers). LMM, light media multitasker; HMM, heavy media multitasker. Source data
Supplementary information
Supplementary Information
This file contains Supplementary Methods, Supplementary Discussion, Supplementary Notes, and Supplementary References.
Rights and permissions
About this article
Cite this article
Madore, K.P., Khazenzon, A.M., Backes, C.W. et al. Memory failure predicted by attention lapsing and media multitasking. Nature (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-020-2870-z
Received31 January 2020
Accepted03 August 2020
Published28 October 2020
Subjects
Comments
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.